How do
male cleaner fish “punishing” female cleaner fish affect the symbiotic
relationship between cleaner fish and other species? What are the costs and rewards of this
distinct behavior? Punishment is a form
of intricate interaction that was considered to be carried out solely by the
human species to alter cooperation in one way or another. However, recent
studies have proven this belief false by observing behavior of a fish
underwater. The fish, Bluestreak Cleaner Wrasse Labroides dimidiatus, has a significantly smaller brain than humans. Labroides dimidiatus, more commonly
referred to as the cleaner wrasse. This unique species of marine fish thrive
off of a symbiotic relationship with their “clients” where they provide
grooming services for other fish species in response to nutrients and evasion
from predation.
Obstacles to this relationship created by outlaw Cleaner fish
causing reduced fitness result in punishment. This species essentially runs its
underwater ‘spa’ while attending to their customers in pairs of male and
female. However, this business does not hold the best reputation in the ocean
as often time, one fish in the pair will tend to cheat and bite off more than
it is expected to. In other words, it takes a chunk off of the client’s mucus
membrane in addition to the ectoparasites. This results in the client swimming
away and reduced fitness for both of the fish, even though only one cheated.
This reduction in fitness leads to punishment where a fish would chase the
criminal aggressively, but strangely, only the female wrasses get punished.
This may have been a result of various evolutionary processes but it leads to
various situations and restrictions regarding fitness, with males ensuring
their position as the reproductive competitors.
Cleaner fish with host damselfish. Photo by Richard Smith Source |
Cleaner wrasses avoid predation by
providing a service to several species of fish in the reef. This relationship
is proven in results of several studies but one of the most significant was
conducted by Karen Cheney of the University of Queensland in Australia. In the
study, a mimic of the juvenile cleaner wrasse Labroides dimidiatus, also known by the name of the Bluestriped
Fangblenny was observed. Even though Fangblennies attack reef fish, they use
mimicry to wrasses to avoid predation. It was found that this species can
change to the mimic coloration within 5-10 minutes depending on the presence of
the wrasse (Cheney et al. 2013). It is definitely a possibility that this mimic
species affects the fitness of the cleaner wrasse in a negative way by making
potential clients wary of the actual cleaner wrasse itself. (Cheney et al. 2013).
The primary and most significant
factor affecting fitness comes from clients. Clients will tend to avoid
stations at which they have not been treated well in the past. This will
greatly reduce fitness of both the male and female since there will be less
nutrients available to them. In a male dominated population, a smaller male
resulting from less nutrients available will have significantly lower
reproductive success when it comes to competition. In certain cases, cheating
by female wrasses will lead to a point where they will get as big in size as
the male wrasses and consequently, change their sex due to their hermaphroditic
nature. Since this species relies on size-based dominance hierarchies, and this
species is able to change sex if it reaches a certain size (With the exception
of the Carribean cleaner), punishment is a crucial factor for males in the
society. The male has to ensure that the female partner does not outgrow him
and change its sex to become a reproductive competitor. In this situation, the
female proceeds to become a competitor with the male wrasses for client
territory.
Bluestreak Cleaner Wrasse with Moray Eel host. |
There have been several scientific experiments carried out on the
behavior of wrasses with their client and how it affects the relationship
between the competing male wrasses, and the wrasse couples that include the
male and female. In one study, pairs of cleaners were exposed to a model client
offering either prawn items which were more preferred, or flake items which
were less preferred and used to imitate ecto-parasites. When the model client
was removed as the consequence of the wrasse eating the prawn items (analogous
to client mucus), the severity of male punishment was based on the actual
pay-off reduction from the client lost. More severe punishments lead to higher
female cooperation only until the female gains advantage in size through the
foraging benefits obtained from cheating. In this inverse relationship, the
bigger the female gets in size through foraging advantages derived from
cheating, the smaller the male gets through two factors: Firstly, the male will
be unable to obtain nutrition from the ecto-parasites and secondly, the male
loses the client from his “spa” forever while the female has already benefited
from cheating. Therefore, this dilemma eventually allows the female to surpass
the male in size and become a reproductive competitor by changing her sex due
to the hermaphroditic nature of the species. Hence, the male has to ensure that
the cost the punishing has on his fitness is worth the punishment so the female
is prevented from cheating again and getting larger in size. (Raihani et al.
2011) Other studies have demonstrated that this relationship is not only a
simple all or none type of punishment cooperation relationship, but more of a
carefully evolved relationship where the amount of punishment is elaborately
calculated in response to the intensity of the payoff lost. Additionally, other
methods of intricate control are also employed by male wrasses depending on the
cheating which include sanctions and partner switching. Results showing
different forms of punishment for different cheating behaviors prove that these
punishment responsive cooperations have developed and adjusted over numerous
amounts of years as a results of evolution when compared to a non-cleaning fish
species Halichoeres melanurus, which
showed no difference in foraging behavior under the same conditions. (Gingins
et al. 2013)
When it comes to collateral effects of cleaner fish on
other species and the coral reef habitat, it was found that cleaner fish are
extremely beneficial to the coral reef habitat and species that it hosts. In a
study carried out on the coral reef, patch reefs with and without cleaner fish
were observed for populations and fitness of client fish. It was found that
individuals in reef patches without the cleaner fish were smaller, 37% less
abundant and 23% less species rich when compared to control reef patches with
the cleaner fish. This demonstrates that cleaner fish behavior has
community-wide effects and is beneficial to client fish. Additional evidence
also suggested that juveniles were 65% less abundant which points to lower
survivorship (Waldie et al. 2011). This evidence proves that the cleaner wrasse
qualifies as a keystone species in a coral reef and impacts the health of all
fish species in the habitat by playing the role of the doctor.
References
Cheney,
K.L. 2013. Cleaner fish coloration
decreases predation risk in aggressive fangblenny mimics. Behavioral
Ecology doi: 10.1093/beheco/art043
Gingins, S., J. Werminghausen, R.A. Johnstone, A.S. Grutter, and R. Bshary. 2013. Power and temptation cause shifts between exploitation and cooperation in a cleaner wrasse mutualism. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2013.0553
Raihani, N J., A.I. Pinto, A.S.
Grutter, S. Wismer, and R. Bshary. 2011.
Male cleaner wrasses adjust punishment of female partners according to the stakes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0690
No comments:
Post a Comment