Reproduction in fish is dependent on a large
number of complex communication factors. The response of the female to male
courtship behavior is largely based on the male’s identity, quality,
motivation, readiness, and social status. Visual
communication, the most studied and best understood mechanism, is just one of
the multiple reproductive sensory mechanisms involved in reproduction. . Female choice often relies on
visual traits such as size, dominance, mechanoreception, and chemoreception. In Burton's Mouthbrooder Astatotilapia burtoni (Günther, 1893), the male does series of
tail waggles that females use as an indicator of their dominance in the
social hierarchy. Females also choose to reproduce with the male based on the acoustic
frequency in combination with a multitude of other factors.
Soundwaves produced by a yellow dominant male during courtship (Maurska et al. 2012) |
There are many factors
that weigh into a female’s decision to reproduce with a male. Size is a
contributor to female reception. A male displaying a certain trait that is
larger (such as a larger caudal fin) may be more attractive in some species
(Bischoff et al. 1985). Some fish are considered dominant based on factors like size of territory
owned or coloration. Dominant males are also more likely to reproduce (Spence 2006). Male
odors can also influence a female's preference (Fisher et al. 2006). Females can also use their
lateral line to either avoid males or court them (Medina et al. 2013). The sexual response of a female Astatotilapia
Burtoni is largely dependent on sounds produced by the male. Many fish use
sound to deter predators and intruders, identify members of the same species,
and to attract mates. Although the Astatotilapia Burtoni uses sounds in its
courtship, it is important to note that not all fish, including many in the
cichlidae family do not. Different sound frequencies may even influence
morphological changes in some sympatric species, leading to reproductive
isolation over time (Longrie et al. 2013).
The likelihood that a female accepts a male as a mate is
correlated with the frequency of the noise that he is emitting. These sounds
are species specific, varying in “trill duration, number of pulses per trill,
pulse period, pulse duration, and interpulse interval” (Maruska et al. 2012). Females in the species Astatotilapia burtoni
use sound to choose their mate based on frequency. This is tested in the paper
written by Maruska, Ung and Fernald. The authors explain, “ we characterized
the sounds and associated behaviors produced by dominant males during
courtship, tested whether there were differences in hearing ability associated
with female reproductive state or male social status, and then tested the
hypothesis that female mate preference is influenced by male sound production.”
(Maruska et al. 2012).
Astatotilpia burtoni has a brightly colored dominant male
phenotype and a normal colored subordinate male phenotype. This makes them easy
to distinguish and easier to use in experiments. The female’s response to courtship depends on the dominance of
the male and the female’s reproductive potential at the time. Females that were
mouthbrooding at the time were unlikely to respond to male courtship behavior.
During courtship, males produce a low frequency sound when close to females. A
female may respond to the male’s courtship differently based on the frequency
of the noise produced. In the experiment from article one, the scientists used
a tank with three compartments. They placed two visually and physically similar
males on the outside compartments of the tank. They then introduced a female to
the center compartment of the tank and played either the natural tail wag sound
or the noise control from speakers in one of the outer compartments. When the
natural sound was played through the speaker, the females spent a greater
portion of their time on that side of the tank. When the unnatural control
sound was played, the female spent equal amounts of time on either side of the
tank. This suggests that females take into account acoustic signals when
choosing a mate. It was determined that the tail wags are associated with
courtship behavior, because “Dominant male A. burtoni produced pulsed broadband
sounds during body quivers associated with courtship behaviors. Our
simultaneous sound and video recordings demonstrate that these courtship sounds
are produced intentionally because not all quiver behaviors were associated
with sound production, suggesting that the sound is not merely a by-product of
body movements, but that males have some control over when and where it is
produced.” (Maruska et al. 2012). The data show that the
female prefers sounds produced by the male to be in a specific frequency
threshold. More dominant males were able to produce sounds at the optimum
hearing range and for a longer period of time.
The increase in the female sex steroid is
correlated with the frequency of the sound produced by the male. According to
Maruska, Ung, and Fernald, “Astatotilapia burtoni was most sensitive to low
frequencies from ∼200–600 Hz, with a best frequency at 200–300 Hz, which
overlaps the spectral content of the courtship sounds produced by dominant
males.” The sex steroids in the female increased when sounds in this range
were played. When the steroids in the female are increased, the female is more
likely to reproduce. The more dominant males are able to produce this sound
while the less dominant males’ sounds are more likely to be ignored (Maruska et al. 2012).
-->
This research is a good indicator that, in combination with other traits
such as size, dominance, mechanoreception and chemoreception, sound is used as
a way to attract mates in the African Cichlid species Astatotilapia burtoni.
During courtship, males produce courtship sounds that are conducive to
increasing female sex steroids. This raises the female’s desire to mate with
males who are more dominant, The experiment proved that the courtship sounds
produced by Astatotilapia burtoni are both deliberate and vital to reproductive
success.
Bischoff, R. J., J. L. Gould, and D. I. Rubenstein. 1985. Tail size and female choice in the Guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 17(3):253-55. doi:10.1007/bf00300143.
Fisher, H. S., B. B.m Wong, and G. G. Rosenthal. 2006. Alteration of the chemical environment disrupts communication in a freshwater fish. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273(1591): 1187-193. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3406.
Longrie, N., P. Poncin, M. Denoël, V. Gennotte, J. Delcourt, and E. Parmentier. 2013. "Behaviours Associated with Acoustic Communication in Nile Tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus)." PLoS ONE 8(4) doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0061467.
Maruska, K. P., U. S. Ung, and R. D. Fernald. 2012. The African Cichlid Fish Astatotilapia burtoni uses acoustic communication for reproduction: sound production, hearing, and behavioral significance." PLoS ONE 7(5):e37612 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037612.
Medina, L.M., C.M. Garcia, A.F. Urbina, J.Manjarrez, and A. Moyaho. 2013. Female vibration discourages male courtship behaviour in the Amarillo Fish (Girardinichthys multiradiatus). Behavioural Processes 100:163-68. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2013.09.007.
Spence, R. 2006. Mating preference of female Zebrafish, Danio rerio, in relation to male dominance. Behavioral Ecology 17(5): 779-83. doi:10.1093/beheco/arl016.
It will found very helpful in order to control industrial noise. Nice information!
ReplyDeleteAcoustics Solution for Meeting Room Perth