A fish must eat or it will not survive long, especially in
the warm ocean waters. We often think of
a fish by its feeding strategy. For
example, the Pickerel and Barracuda are camouflaged sit-and-wait predators, the
Goosefish also sits and waits, but it has a lure to attract prey, sturgeons are benthic foragers, and Menhaden, Sardines, and Whale Sharks are filter
feeders. The morphology of each fish is
specialized to facilitate its dominant feeding strategies. But the Grouper and
the Moray Eel often hunt together to increase prey captures. This cooperative hunting behavior in two
unrelated carnivores was first observed by Bshary and associates in the Red
Sea. It may be the first reported occurrence of cooperative hunting by unrelated species.
The Roving Coralgrouper, Plectropomus
pessuliferus Fowler, 1904, is a large grouper (Serranidae, Epinephelinae,
Epinephelini) that roams on the reefs in the
Indo-Pacific Ocean and reaches 1.2 m. The
groupers are a highly photogenic marine fishes. All members of this genus have
the same dorsal fin formula “VIII, 11” and three antrorse spines on the lower margin of the
preopercle. All seven species of Plectropomus possess the beautiful
pattern of blue spots on the head, body, and median fins (see photo). Roving Coralgroupers usually live and hunt
alone. Only during the breeding season,
do they join other Roving Coralgroupers in spawning aggregations. This grouper is most likely a
protogynous hermaphrodite, like other close relatives. The Roving Coralgrouper is a diurnal
predator with a large mouth. These
large-bodied rover predators use their burst speed and vacuum action of the
large buccal cavity to capture prey in open water. Consequently, its prefers to eat other
fishes, typically damselfishes (Pomacentridae), wrasses(Labridae), and anything else that moves too slow.
Photo of Roving Coralgrouper Plectropomus pessuliferu. Photo by Pere Rubio |
Giant Moray Eel Gymnothorax javanicus. Photo by Andrew Bruckner |
The fascinating study by Redouan Bshary and associates, of University
of Neuchâtel, Switzerland, described cooperative hunting behavior of the Roving
Coralgrouper and the Giant Moray Eel. By cooperation we mean that the two species
communicate regarding their needs and cooperate to help each other capture more
prey. Further, true cooperation occurs
only when individuals play different roles during a hunt. How do these two
species communicate? If the grouper is
hungry, s/he will approach with head shaking (3-6 shakes/second) directly in
front of the Giant Moray’s head. This
gesture has been referred to as the “shimmy signal.” The spiny dorsal fin is always depressed. Apparently,
this signal means “let’s go hunting together.”
The moray understands the signal
and the two fish swim off together (58% of the observations). The grouper does the head shake behavior
while performing a headstand over the hiding place of its prey. This
signals to the moray to investigate the crevice, which they do. Because the moray flushes the prey, the groupers
caught almost five times as many prey items per unit time than when morays were
absent. Sometimes they observed moray
eels catch prey when hunting with a grouper, but the two species never
displayed any aggression when hunting.
The Red Sea investigators documented many interactions
between the two fish species; 35% of these were 2 minutes or longer (up to 44
minutes). Clearly, this was not a
random occurrence. The Giant Moray
stayed within 1-2 grouper body lengths during the interaction. Both hunting buddies increased their
feeding success when hunting cooperatively.
No cheating is possible because both of these fishes swallow their prey
whole, leaving no trailing parts to “fight” over.
Other SCUBA divers have subsequently videotaped this
cooperative hunting behavior. The behaviors of these hunting partners were videotaped by Bshary and his coauthors. See here. However, others have also witnessed and videotaped the behavior, including FrederikW, Karel Mestdagh, and David Whitehead101.
The grouper and moray are two unlikely hunting buddies that
participate in true cooperative behavior, in which both partners benefit from
the association. The head-shaking and
head-down behavior of the Roving Coralgrouper bears all the characteristics of
an intentional “referential gesture.” It communicates the location of the hidden
prey to the hunting partner. The joint
hunting expeditions are more successful because the adaptations of the two fish
are complementary. The intentionality of a gesture is a characteristic of communication among primates and has been
seldom investigated in fishes (Vail et al. 2013). However, this behavior suggests that cognitive
processes may underly the gesture. We
have no simple way to ask sophisticated questions on cognition in wild,
free-living fishes.
References
Bshary, R., A. Hohner, K. Ait-el-Duoudi, and H. Fricke. 2006. Interspecific
communicative and coordinated hunting between groupers and Giant Moray Eels in
the Red Sea. PLoS Biology 4(12):
e431. DOI:
10.1371/journal.pbio.0040431
Vail, A.L., A. Manica, and R. Bshary. 2013. Referential gestures in fish collaborative
hunting. Nature Communications 4:1765 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms2781
Without regulations, anyone could hunt deer which means the entire experience and situation becomes very dangerous. Keep in mind that while deer hunting regulations are similar from one state to another, some variances do exist. Click here
ReplyDeleteThe 2014 season is just getting underway, and yet some hunters will have not even checked their gear, got some practice in, or thought about where they're going to hunt. Like anything else, and even perhaps more so with bow hunting, you get out what you put in. huntingmastery
ReplyDeleteGreat post.
ReplyDeletehttp://ttlink.com/andyroma/detail